Type: Package
Title: Open Datasets from Meta-Analyses in Psychology Research
Version: 1.0.1
Description: Data and examples from meta-analyses in psychology research.
License: GPL-2 | GPL-3 [expanded from: GPL (≥ 2)]
Encoding: UTF-8
LazyData: true
RoxygenNote: 7.1.2
Imports: Rdpack
RdMacros: Rdpack
NeedsCompilation: no
Packaged: 2022-03-23 17:20:31 UTC; josue
Author: Josue E. Rodriguez [aut, cre], Donald Williams [aut]
Maintainer: Josue E. Rodriguez <josue.rodriguez594@gmail.com>
Depends: R (≥ 3.5.0)
Repository: CRAN
Date/Publication: 2022-03-23 17:40:02 UTC

Studies on Out-Group Entitativity and Prejudice

Description

Results from 21 studies, including 85 effect sizes (fisher-z), on the effect of out-Group entitativity and prejudice (Agadullina and Lovakov 2018).

Usage

data("agadullina2018")

Format

A data frame with 85 rows and 9 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://osf.io/8dw5y/.

References

Agadullina ER, Lovakov AV (2018). “Are people more prejudiced towards groups that are perceived as coherent? A meta-analysis of the relationship between out-group entitativity and prejudice.” British Journal of Social Psychology, 57(4), 703–731.


Studies on the cognitive and academic benefits of Cogmed

Description

Results from 48 studies, including 637 effect sizes (Hedge's g), on the effect of the Cogmed Working Memory Training program on cognitive and academic outcomes (Aksayli et al. 2019).

Usage

data(aksayli2019)

Format

A dataset with 637 rows and 15 variables.

Source

https://osf.io/jhavp/

References

Aksayli ND, Sala G, Gobet F (2019). “The cognitive and academic benefits of Cogmed: A meta-analysis.” Educational Research Review, 27, 229–243.


Studies on Math Anxiety and Math Achievement

Description

Results from 332 studies, including 747 effect sizes in total (Fisher-z), on the relation between math anxiety and math achievement (Barroso et al. 2021).

Usage

data("barroso2021")

Format

A data frame with 747 rows and 11 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://osf.io/5admx/.

References

Barroso C, Ganley CM, McGraw AL, Geer EA, Hart SA, Daucourt MC (2021). “A meta-analysis of the relation between math anxiety and math achievement.” Psychological Bulletin, 147(2), 134.


Studies on the Facial Feedback Literature

Description

Results from 138 studies, including 274] effect sizes (Cohen's d), on the facial feedback hypothesis (Coles et al. 2019).

Usage

data(coles2019)

Format

A dataset with 286 rows and 13 variables.

Source

https://osf.io/v8kxb/

References

Coles NA, Larsen JT, Lench HC (2019). “A meta-analysis of the facial feedback literature: Effects of facial feedback on emotional experience are small and variable.” Psychological bulletin, 145(6), 610.


Meta-analytic data collected from studying on the specificity of future thinking in depression

Description

Results from 46 studies, including 89 effect sizes (r), on the specificity of future thinking in depression (Gamble et al. 2019)

Usage

data(gamble2019)

Format

A data frame with 89 rows and 20 variables.

Source

https://osf.io/5wjb2/

References

Gamble B, Moreau D, Tippett LJ, Addis DR (2019). “Specificity of future thinking in depression: A meta-analysis.” Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(5), 816–834.


Studies on the Color Red and Cognitive Performance

Description

Results from 22 studies, including 67 effect sizes (SMD), on the effect of the color red on cognitive performance (Gnambs 2020).

Usage

data("gnambs2020")

Format

A data frame with 67 rows and 10 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://psyarxiv.com/a4qdv/.

References

Gnambs T (2020). “Limited evidence for the effect of red color on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis.” Psychonomic bulletin \& review, 27(6), 1374–1382.


Studies on the advantage of bilingualism in children: a meta-analytic review

Description

Results from 150 studies, including 1194 effect sizes (Hedge's g), on the extent to which shared reading impacts language development (Lowe 2020).

Usage

data("lowe2020")

Format

A data frame with 1194 rows and 20 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://osf.io/jv7wt/

References

Lowe C (2020). “The bilingual advantage in children: a meta-analytic review.” PsyArXiv.


Studies examining whether student emotional intelligence is associated with academic performance

Description

Results from 158 studies, including 1246 effect sizes (r), on the relationship between emotional intelligence (EI) and academic performance (MacCann et al. 2020).

Usage

data("maccann2020")

Format

A data frame with 1246 rows and 19 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://osf.io/hnmy4/

References

MacCann C, Jiang Y, Brown LE, Double KS, Bucich M, Minbashian A (2020). “Emotional intelligence predicts academic performance: A meta-analysis.” Psychological Bulletin, 146(2), 150.


Studies on Age Differences in Executive Functioning

Description

Results from 431 studies, including 1268 effect sizes (Hedge's g), on age differences in executive functioning (Maldonado et al. 2020).

Usage

data("maldonado2020")

Format

A data frame with 1268 rows and 13 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://osf.io/bcywg/.

References

Maldonado T, Orr JM, Goen JR, Bernard JA (2020). “Age differences in the subcomponents of executive functioning.” The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 75(6), e31–e55.


Studies from ManyBabies 1: Infant-Directed Speech Preference.

Description

Meta-analytic data collected from the ManyBabies Consortium, including 108 effect sizes, aimed at assessing the overall replicability of theoretically-important phenomenon and examing the methodological, situational, cultural, and developmental moderators on infant's preference for infant-directed speech (IDS) over adult-directed speech (ADS) ⁠ ⁠

Usage

data(manybabies2020)

Format

A dataset with 108 rows and 8 variables.

Source

https://github.com/manybabies/mb1-analysis-public

References

ManyBabiesConsortium (2020). “Quantifying sources of variability in infancy research using the infant-directed-speech preference.” Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 3(1), 24–52.


Studies from the Many Labs 2 project.

Description

A subset of the data collected in the Many Labs 2 project which conducted replications of 28 classic and contemporary findings in psychology. The study examined the extent to which variability in replication success can be attributed to the study sample.

Usage

data(manylabs2018)

Format

A dataset with 1,414 rows and 23 variables.

Source

https://osf.io/ux3eh/

References

Klein, R. A., et al. (2018). Many Labs 2: Investigating variation in replicability across samples and settings. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(4), 443-490. (APS)


Studies on Shared Reading and Language Development

Description

Results from 54 studies, including 316 effect sizes (Hedge's g), on the extent to which shared reading impacts language development (Noble et al. 2019).

Usage

data("noble2019")

Format

A data frame with 316 rows and 13 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://osf.io/34xyw/

References

Noble C, Sala G, Peter M, Lingwood J, Rowland C, Gobet F, Pine J (2019). “The impact of shared book reading on children's language skills: A meta-analysis.” Educational Research Review, 28, 100290.


Data collected from meta-analyses on intelligence research

Description

Data resulting from 131 meta-analyses, including 2443 effect sizes (fisher-z), on different areas of intelligence research (Nuijten et al. 2020)

Usage

data(nuijten2020)

Format

A dataset with 2443 rows and 14 variables.

Source

https://osf.io/fq5wp/

References

Nuijten MB, van Assen MA, Augusteijn HE, Crompvoets EA, Wicherts JM (2020). “Effect sizes, power, and biases in intelligence research: A meta-meta-analysis.” Journal of Intelligence, 8(4), 36.


Studies on the impact of working-memory training on near- and far-transfer measures

Description

Results from 332 studies, including 1,555 effect sizes (Hedge's g), on whether skills learned from cognitive training generalize to other situations (Sala et al. 2019).

Usage

data(sala2019)

Format

A data frame with 1,555 rows and 10 variables:

* study_id: unique id for each meta-analysis * es_id: unique id for each effect size * yi: the observed effect size, expressed in Hedge's g * vi: the variance of the observed effect size * ni: the total sample size for the observed effect size in the meta-analysis * author: author of study * comparison: type of control group ("Active" or "Non-active") * age: Age group used in study ("adults", "LD children", "TD children", "old", or "children") * test: test used in study * model: indicator for which model the study is used (see paper for details)

Source

https://osf.io/qk2vu/

References

Sala G, Aksayli ND, Tatlidil KS, Tatsumi T, Gondo Y, Gobet F, Zwaan R, Verkoeijen P (2019). “Near and far transfer in cognitive training: A second-order meta-analysis.” Collabra: Psychology, 5(1).


Studies on the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on inhibitory control

Description

Results from 62 studies, including 75 effect sizes (Hedge's g) on the effect of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in inhibitory control (Schroeder et al. 2020).

Usage

data("schroeder2020")

Format

A data frame with 75 rows and 13 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://osf.io/mrxhe/

References

Schroeder PA, Schwippel T, Wolz I, Svaldi J (2020). “Meta-analysis of the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on inhibitory control.” Brain Stimulation.


Studies on Executive function components in intellectual disability

Description

Results from 26 studies, including 99 effect sizes (Hedge's g), on inhibition, shifting, and attention in people with intellectual disability compared to people matched on mental age (Spaniol and Danielsson 2019).

Usage

data("spaniol2020")

Format

A data frame with 99 rows and 11 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://psyarxiv.com/gjqcs/

References

Spaniol M, Danielsson H (2019). “A Meta-analysis of the Executive Functions Inhibition, Shifting and Updating in Intellectual Disabilities.” PsyArXiv.


Studies on the association between goal orientation and performance adaptation

Description

Results from 35 studies, including 76 effect sizes (r), on learning goal orientation and perfromance adaptation (Stasielowicz 2019).

Usage

data("stasielowicz2019a")

Format

A data frame with 76 rows and 24 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://osf.io/szfwx/

References

Stasielowicz L (2019). “Goal orientation and performance adaptation: A meta-analysis.” Journal of Research in Personality, 82, 103847.


Studies on the association between goal orientation and performance adaptation

Description

Results from 28 studies, including 86 effect sizes (r), on performance goal orientation and performance adaptation (Stasielowicz 2019).

Usage

data("stasielowicz2019b")

Format

A data frame with 86 rows and 25 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://osf.io/szfwx/

References

Stasielowicz L (2019). “Goal orientation and performance adaptation: A meta-analysis.” Journal of Research in Personality, 82, 103847.


Studies on the importance of cognitive ability in performance adaptation

Description

Results from 47 independent samples, including 133 effect sizes (r), on the role of individual differences in cognitive abilities in the context of performance adaption (Stasielowicz 2020).

Usage

data("stasielowicz2020")

Format

A data frame with 133 rows and 23 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://psyarxiv.com/qu4t2/

References

Stasielowicz L (2020). “How important is cognitive ability when adapting to changes? A meta-analysis of the performance adaptation literature.” Personality and Individual Differences, 166, 110178.


Studies on Social Identity Theory and Leadership: Leader Group Prototypicality

Description

Results from 128 studies, including 251 effect sizes (fisher-z), on the extent to which a leader is perceived to embody shared social identity (Steffens et al. 2021).

Usage

data("steffens2020")

Format

A data frame with 251 rows and 10 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://osf.io/y47er/

References

Steffens NK, Munt KA, van Knippenberg D, Platow MJ, Haslam SA (2021). “Advancing the social identity theory of leadership: A meta-analytic review of leader group prototypicality.” Organizational Psychology Review, 11(1), 35–72.


Studies on memory suppression and its deficiency in psychological disorders

Description

Results from 25 studies, including 96 effect sizes (Cohen's d), on suppression-induced forgetting (Stramaccia et al. 2020).

Usage

data("stramaccia2021")

Format

A data frame with 96 rows and 15 variables:

Details

Further details can be found at https://osf.io/f89ur/

References

Stramaccia DF, Meyer A, Rischer KM, Fawcett JM, Benoit RG (2020). “Memory suppression and its deficiency in psychological disorders: A focused meta-analysis.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.


Studies on juvenile recidivism

Description

Results from 17 studies, including 100 effect sizes (Cohen's D) on the associations between mental health disorders of delinquent juveniles and subsequent delinquent behavior

Usage

data(wibbelink2017)

Format

A data frame with 100 rows and 10 variables.

Source

The Quantitative Methods in Psychology

References

Wibbelink et al. (2017). A meta-analysis of the association between mental health disorders and juvenile recidivism. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 33, 78-90.